Willem H. J. Martens, MD, PhD
Director of the “W. Kahn Institute of Theoretical Psychiatry and Neuroscience.”
Beatrixstraat 45, 3921 BN Elst
Utrecht, The Netherlands
WimmartensW@netscape.net.
Although psychopaths demonstrate emotional
abnormalities such as shallow affect, lack of empathy, incapacity for love,
lack of guilt or remorse, lack of fear, and emotional processing and response
deficiencies they may show normal emotional responses or emotional
hypersensitive in other areas. The correlates of emotional incapacities,
emotional hypersensitivity, and normal emotional activities in psychopaths are
studied and discussed in this paper. Emotional hypersensitivity might be linked
with: a history of neglect, rejection and abuse; insult; changes which are
forced or not under control of the psychopath; obstacles that prevent the
psychopath to do what he or she wants to do; narcissistic injury; broken
friendships or relationship. Normal emotional functioning might be associate
with grief, warm relationship, adequate attention, disease, academic and/or
occupational success, impressive events, confrontations, contemplation and
maturation, hidden suffering (also as a result of neurobiological
determination).
1. Introduction
According to Cleckley (1984) the psychopath is able to reproduce
the “pantomime” of emotions, without experiencing the emotions itself. But, the
author suggests that adequate
reproduction of pantomime of emotions requires understanding of the
meaning and logic of emotions in specific contexts, and that this is quiet
remarkable for someone who is not experiencing the emotions itself. The
processing of emotional expressions is fundamental for normal socialization and
interaction. Adequate emotional functioning requires a)
self-consciousness, b) a Self that evaluates changes by means of
self-observation and reflection, and c) a cognitive ability to observe,
discern, associate, compare, and revoke (Hales et al., 1994). Emotional condition refers
to internal processes, which are connected with somatic and physiological
activity. It correlates also with observable changes in face, voice, body and
activity levels that occurs when the central nervous system is activated by
emotional salient stimuli (Lewis & Michelson, 1983). Psychopathy is
characterized by emotional deficiencies and correlated neurobiological
dysfunctions (Martens, 2002), and abnormal internal and intrapsychic processes (Martens, 1997,
2002c), and severe socialization problems (Martens, 2000; Cleckley, 1984). Martens (1997, 2000) revealed that
a history of rejection; neglect; physical and sexual abuse; parental antisocial
behavior, - substance abuse, and - divorce; adoption; and a bad and unsafe
neighborhood might be linked to emotional deficits in psychopaths. It is argued
that different forms of aggressive emotions that are significant in psychopaths
may be related to disparate facets of psychopathy such as impulsivity,
irritability, social-emotional, and moral incapacities (Martens, 2003a), and
that these relationships may be mediated by common dispositional factors (Patrick
& Zempolich, 1998) such as negative experiences in childhood and
adolescence (Martens, 1997) and neurobiological and genetic determination
(Martens, 2002a).
Eidemiller & Yustitky (1987) discovered that following factors
may contribute to abnormal emotional development in juvenile psychopaths a)
expansion of the sphere of parental emotions, b) preference for the child
qualities in an adolescent, c) lack of educational self-confidence, d) parental
fear of losing the child, e) underdeveloped parental feelings, f) projection of
the parent's unwanted qualities onto the adolescent, and g) introduction of
spousal conflicts into the educational sphere. Moreover, the emergence of
abnormal moral emotions later in life may be linked to temperament and
attachment deficiencies in early childhood of the psychopath (Saltaris, 2002).
Psychopathic personality disorder (PPD) has some overlap with antisocial personality disorder (ASPD) according the DSM-IV criteria of the American Psychiatric Association (1994), which is nowadays the official term, but these disorders are not synonymous (Martens, 2000). ASPD (DSM-IV, 1994) and PPD (Cleckley, 1984; Hare et al., 1990; Hare, 1991) is characterized by features like irritability and aggressiveness, impulsivity or failure to planning ahead, reckless disregard for the safety of self and others, pathological egocentricity, lack of guilt or remorse, social maladjustment, poor development of relationships, deceitfulness. Criminality is included in the criteria of ASPD but not of PPD. Furthermore, psychopathic personality disorder is characterized by following traits which do not meet ASPD criteria according the DSM-IV: inadequate motivated antisocial behavior; lack of nervousness and psychoneurotic manifestations; absence of delusions and other signs of irrational thinking; superficial charm and good intelligence; incapacity for love; specific loss of insight; unresponsiveness in general interpersonal relations; fantastic and uninviting behavior with drink and sometimes without; suicide rarely carried out; sex life impersonal, trivial, and poorly integrated; poor judgment and failure to learn from experiences (Cleckley, 1984); manipulative behavior; conning; pathological lying; grandiose sense of self-worth; need for stimulation/ proneness to boredom; shallow affect and/or callousness and lack of empathy; parasitic life-style; poor self-control; promiscuous sexual behavior; many short-term marital relationships; early behavioral problems juvenile criminality and versatility among others (Hare, 1991); poor fear conditioning (lack of fear) (Lykken 1995) and sensation seeking (Zuckerman, 1994); good reality testing ( Dorr & Woodhall, 1986; Fingarette, 1972; Martens, 1997; Wolman, 1987), and hidden suffering (Martens, 2002b).
Although the precise nature of emotional deficits in ASPD and PPD is unknown (Herpertz & Sass, 2000, Herpertz et al., 2001; Martens, 1997), research indicated that these patients demonstrate abnormal emotional functioning and processing (Habel et al., 2002; Steuerwald & Kosson, 2000) such as:
· Lack of empathic emotions (Cleckley, 1984, Hare, 1991; Lewis, 1991; Martens, 1997). Empathy impairment (Blair et al., 2001b) and other (related) affective abnormalities (Kiehl et al., 2001) in psychopaths may be linked to amygdala deficiencies (Blair et al., 2001b), or to deficient or weakened input from limbic structures (Kiehl et al., 2001);
· Inappropriate emotions (Cleckley, 1984; Lewis, 1991; Martens, 1997);
· A pronounced lack of fear in response to aversive events (Herpertz et al., 2000, 2001; Martens, 2000; Patrick, 1994), which may correlate with low autonomic activity/reactivity (Blair et al., 1997; Fowles, 2000; Martens, 2000), and lesions of the dorsal hippocampus (Laakso et al., 2001);
· Difficulties in emotional processing and expressions. Psychopaths demonstrate poor control of emotional expression (Fowles, 2000), general deficit in processing affective information, regardless of whether stimuli are negative or positive (Christianson et al., 1996; Herpertz et al., 2001; Patrick et al., 1993) that manifests itself independently of affective report (Patrick et al., 1993). They show also affective imaginary deficiencies (Patrick et al., 1993, 1994), and lower generalized emotional responsivity (Day & Wong, 1996; Sutton et al., 2002) in comparison with normal controls. Sonderstrom et al., (2002) revealed neurologic determined (such as reduced frontotemporal perfusion) affective unresponsiveness (Sonderstrom et al., 2002) and/or psychophysiological determined (such as ventromedial prefrontal dysfunction) diminished emotionality (Barrash et al., 2000).
o Specific deficits in nonverbal emotional processing (Kosson et al., 2002). Psychopaths show less facial expression (Herpertz et al., 2001), and reduced responsiveness to the facial expressions of sadness and fear has been implicated in the development of psychopathy (Blair et al., 2001b; Stevens et al., 2001).
o “semantic dementia,” which is characterized by a discordance between the language values and experiential values of emotions. According to this concept, psychopaths would have an appropriate cognitive representation of the lexical meaning of emotions, but not the affective value normally attached to them (Johns and Quay, 1962). Semantic and (associated) emotional processes are dissociated in psychopaths (Patrick, 1994; Patrick et al., 1994). Kiehl et al., (1999b) revealed that psychopathy is associated with abnormal processing of semantic and affective verbal information. Louth (1998) and Blair et al. (2002) discovered that psychopaths did not differentiate, in voice emphasis, between neutral and affective words. These results could be interpreted with reference to the low-fear and violence inhibition mechanism models of psychopathy (Blair et al., 2002). Furthermore, these findings are consistent with the developing view that psychopaths are insensitive to the emotional connotations of language. In addition, their vocal characteristics may be part of a self-presentation mode designed to manipulate and control interpersonal interactions (Louth et al., 1998). Williamson et al. (1991) discovered that psychopaths extract less information from affective words than do other individuals (Williamson et al., 1991). They show also lower word responsively (Day & Wong, 1996), their lexical decisions were relatively unaffected by emotion cues, and their lexical decisions were relatively unaffected by affectively neutral word-frequency cues compared with non-psychopaths (Lorenz & Newman, 2002).
· a lack of capacity for self-dialogue (Miller, 1987; Martens, 1997), which may have adverse consequences for a) the development of morality and conscience and associated social-emotional awareness, understanding and abilities, b) evaluation of self and other people’s behavior/intentions, and emotions c) the creation of realistic image of Self and other’s, d) utilization of feedback of other people, e) self-control;
·
Emotional
deficiency which may predispose to violence in several ways (Herpertz &
Sass, 2000). The author suggests that abnormal emotional functioning may be
linked to significant correlates of violence such as lack of empathy,
indifferent attitude, disregard of rights and safety of others, lack of
emotional control and associated rage, common neurobiological determination of
emotional deficiencies and violence (Martens, 2002a).
The author hypothesizes that emotional incapacities and/or shallow emotions may be the result of negative, painful experiences in the past and de-sensibilization in order to avoid further excessive suffering. In psychopaths in remission a normalization of emotional functioning (and probably associated neurobiological correlates) can be observed (Martens, 1997, 2002b). The author speculates that the patient is able to influence his or her neurobiological functioning in order to adapt it to his or her emotional needs (Martens, 2001c). Furthermore, research in remitted psychopaths discovered that emotional maturation is linked to other dimensions of maturation (social-emotional, moral, spiritual development, and growth of self-insight and authenticity), which may interrelate with relevant neurobiological processes that determine emotional capacities (Martens, 1997, 2001a, 2002b, 2003c, 2003e).
Although psychopathic personality disorder is characterized by emotional deficits, there is sufficient evidence that psychopaths can be emotionally normal and/or even very sensitive in specific areas, indeed.
Hypersensitivity in psychopaths might be the result of:
Normal, healthy sensitivity may be brought about by:
Psychopathy is diagnostically characterized by serious emotional deficiencies, which may interrelate with other diagnostic features such as incapacitiy for love, lack of empathy, shallow emotions, social-emotional incapacities (lack of interactional skills), pathological egocentricity, grandiose sense of self-worth, irresponsibility, impulsivity, and aggression. Despite their disturbed emotional world some psychopaths may exhibit normal emotional experiences such as normal feelings for pets, relatives, art, sports, and so on. In current and past studies the healthy aspects of emotional life in psychopaths are underexposed. It is, however of major interest to examine the etiological, psychosocial, neurobiological correlates, conditions of normal emotional functioning in different categories of psychopaths, and in what particular circumstances it can happen and flourish. It could be useful for the psychotherapist to direct towards these healthy emotional elements and try to expand or relate them to other emotional and related social and moral areas.
Emotions are hardly objectively measurable, and it is possible that psychopaths “emotional incapacities” in some cases could be better explained as fundamental different emotional functioning rather than emotionally “inferior” or affectively “cold.” Furthermore, there is some evidence that not all “pure” psychopaths demonstrate abnormal emotionality (Martens, 1997). Clear psychopaths (PCL-R scores between 30 and 40) do not necessarily meet all diagnostic features (Hare, 1991), and may show normal emotions. And some psychopaths report that they have normal emotional experiences, while they are unable to show affections (Martens, 1997). This may lead to observations of "shallow" emotions.
More research is needed into effective therapeutic stimulation of emotional development in distinctive categories psychopaths, such as violent non-sexual, violent sexual, frauds, and non-violent and non-criminal psychopaths, which are characterized by their own specific emotional abnormalities which are linked to their crimes and/or personality/behavior patterns (Martens, 1997).
· Barrash J, Tranel D, Anderson SW. Acquired personality disturbances associated with bilateral damage to the ventromedial prefrontal region. Developmental Neuropsychology 2000; 18(3): 355-81.
·
Blair
RJ, Colledge E, Murray L, Mitchell DG. A selective impairment in the processing
of sad and fearful expressions in children with psychopathic tendencies.
Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 2001b; 29(6): 491-8.
·
Blair RJ, Jones L, Clark F, Smith M. The psychopathic
individual: a lack of responsiveness to
distress cues? Psychophysiology. 1997; 34(2): 192-8.
·
Blair RJR,
Mitchell DGV, Richell RA, Kelly S, Leonard A, Newman C, Scott SK. Turning a
deaf ear to fear: Impaired recognition of vocal affect in psychopathic
individuals. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2002; 111(4): 682-686.
·
Cleckley H. Mask of
Sanity. C.V. Mosby: St. Louis, 1984.
· Conacher GN, Fleming RL. The extreme regressive reaction of a psychopath. Psychiatric Quarterly 1996; 67(1): 1-10.
·
Day R, Wong S. Anomalous perceptual asymmetries for
negative emotional stimuli in the psychopath. Journal Abnormal Psychology 1996; 105(4): 648-52.
·
Dorr D, Woodhall P.
Ego dysfunctions in psychopathic psychiatric inpatients. In: Reid WH, Dorr
· D, Walker JI, Bonner JW, Unmasking the psychopath: antisocial personality and related syndromes. New York: WW Norton, 1986, pp. 98-131.
·
Eidemiller EG, Yustitsky VV. Psychological personality problems in
patients as a source of abnormalities in the family upbringing of adolescents
with psychopathic formations.
·
Trudy
Leningradskogo Nauchno Issledovatel'skogo Psikhonevrologicheskogo Instituta im VM Bekhtereva. 1987; 116: 51-55.
· Figarette H. The meaning of criminal insanity. University of Carlifornia: University of California Press, 1972.
·
Fowles DC. Electrodermal hyporeactivity and antisocial
behavior: does anxiety mediate the relationship? Journal Affective Disorders
2000; 61(3): 177-89.
·
Habel U,
Kuehn E, Salloum JB, Devos H, Schneider F. Emotional processing in psychopathic
personality. Aggressive Behavior 2002; Vol 28(5): 394-400.
· Hales RE, Yudofsky SC, Talbott JA. Textbook of psychiatry. Washington, DC: The American Psychiatric Press, 1994.
· Hare RD (1991). The Hare Psychopathy Checklist-Revised. Toronto: Multi-Health Systems.
·
Herpertz SC, Sass H. Emotional deficiency and
psychopathy. Behavioral Sciences and the Law 2000; 18(5): 567-80.
·
Herpertz
SC, Werth U, Lukas G, Qunaibi M, Schuerkens A, Kunert HJ, Freese R, Flesch M,
Mueller-Isberner R, Osterheider M, Sass H. Emotion in criminal offenders with
· psychopathy and borderline personality disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2001; 58(8): 737-45.
· Kiehl KA, Hare RD, McDonald JJ. Brink J. Semantic and affective processing in psychopaths: an event-related potential (ERP) study. Psychophysiology. 1999b; 36(6): 765-74.
· Kiehl KA, Smith AM, Hare RD, Mendrek A, Forster BB, Brink J, Liddle PF. Limbic abnormalities in affective processing by criminal psychopaths as revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Biological Psychiatry 2001; 50(9): 677-84.
·
Kosson DS,
Suchy Y, Mayer AR, Libby J. Facial affect recognition in criminal
psychopaths.Emotion. 2002; Vol 2(4): 398-411.
· Laakso MP, Vaurio O, Koivisto E, Savolainen L, Eronen M, Aronen HJ, Hakola P, Repo E, Soininen H, Tiihonen J. Psychopathy and the posterior hippocampus. Behavioural Brain Research 2001; 118(2): 187-93.
· Lewis CE. Neurochemical mechanisms of chronic antisocial behavior (psychopathy). A literature review. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 1991; 179(12): 720-7.
· Lewis M, Michelson L. Children’s emotions and moods: developmental theory and measurement. New York: Plenum, 1983.
· Lorenz AR, Newman JP. Deficient response modulation and emotion processing in low-anxious Caucasian psychopathic offenders: Results from a lexical decision task. Emotion. 2002; 2(2): 91-104.
· Louth SM, Williamson S, Alpert M, Pouget ER, Hare RD. Acoustic distinctions in the speech of male psychopaths. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 1998; 27(3): 375-84.
· Martens WHJ. Antisocial and psychopathic personality disorders: causes, course and remission – A review article. International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology 2000; 44(4): 406-430.
· Martens WHJ. A theoretical framework of ethics therapy as a distinctive forensic therapeutic
· specialization. International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology 2001a; 45(3): 383-394.
· Martens WHJ. Agitation therapy for antisocial and psychopathic personalities: an outline. American Journal of Psychotherapy 2001b; 55(2): 234-50.
·
Martens WHJ.
Effects of antisocial and social
attitudes on neurobiological functions. Medical Hypotheses 2001c; 56(6):
664-671.
·
Martens WHJ.
Criminality and moral dysfunctions: neurologic, biochemical and genetic
dimensions. International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative
Criminology 2002a; 46(2): 170-182.
·
Martens WHJ. The
hidden suffering of the psychopath. Psychiatric Times 2002b; 19(1): 1-7.
· Martens WHJ. Mental disorders as possible intrapsychic routes to remission. Part II: Psychopathic personality disorder Medical Hypotheses 2002c; 58(6): 509-512.
·
Martens WHJ.
Spiritual psychotherapy for antisocial and psychopathic patients: some
building-blocks. Journal of Contemporary Psychotherapy 2003c (In Press).
·
Martens WHJ.
Structured fantasy therapy for antisocial and psychopathic patients. Journal of
Clinical Psychology 2003d (Under Review).
· Martens WHJ. The art of sublimation: transforming antisocial into social features. American Journal of Psychotherapy 2003e (Under Review).
· Masters B. The shrine of Jeffrey Dahmer. London: Coronet Books/Hodder and Stroughton, 1993.
· Patrick CJ. Emotion and psychopathy: startling new insights. Psychophysiology.1994; 31(4): 319-30.
· Patrick CJ, Bradley MM, Lang PJ. Emotion in the criminal psychopath: startle reflex modulation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 1993; 102(1): 82-92.
· Patrick CJ, Cuthbert BN, Lang PJ. Emotion in the criminal psychopath: fear image processing. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 1994; 103(3): 523- 34.
·
Saltaris C.
Psychopathy in juvenile offenders: Can temperament and attachment be considered
as robust developmental precursors? Clinical Psychology Review. 2002; Vol
22(5): 729-752.
·
Soderstrom H, Hultin L, Tullberg M, Wikkelso C, Ekholm S Forsman A. Reduced
frontotemporal perfusion in psychopathic personality. Psychiatry Research 2002;
114(2): 81-94.
·
Steuerwald BL, Kosson DS. Emotional experiences of the psychopath. In: Gacono CB, Ed, (2000). The clinical and
forensic assessment of psychopathy: A
practitioner's guide. The LEA series in
personality and clinical psychology. Mahwah, NJ, US: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates, Publishers, 2000, pp. 111-135.
·
Sutton SK, Vitale JE, Newman JP. Emotion among women with psychopathy
during picture perception. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 2002;111(4):
610-619.
· Williamson S, Harpur TJ, Hare RD. Abnormal processing of affective words by psychopaths. Psychophysiology. 1991 May; 28(3): 260-73
· Wolman BB. The sociopathic personality. New York: Brunner/Mazel, 1987.
· Zuckerman M. Behavioral expressions and biosocial bases of sensation seeking. New York: Cambridge University Press, 1994.